|
|
Genetic Analysis of Variation in Neuron Number Richelle Cutler
Strom
Chapter 1: INTRODUCTION
Throughout mammalian evolution brain size has varied from a massive 6800
gm in whales down to a meager 0.10 gm in shrews (Count, 1947; Jerison, 1973)
yet the regional organization has remained virtually the same (Butler and
Hodos, 1996). Indeed, upon microscopic examination of the mouse and human
brain Ramon y Cajal, a Nobel Laureate recognized for his detailed
descriptions of the nervous system, stated: "In my opinion there are only
quantitative differences, not qualitative differences between the brain of a
mouse and that of a man." The genetic differences responsible for the vast
differences in brain size are unknown.
Differences in brain size between species ultimately depend on the
accumulation of micro-evolutionary changes among the individuals within a
species (Butler and Hodos, 1996). Variation within a species can be
substantial. For example, normal variation in brain weight among humans
ranges two-fold, from 900 to 1,800 gm (Cobb, 1965). Even among closely
related strains of inbred laboratory mice brain weight differs by as much as
13%, from 429 mg in C3H/HeJ to 490 mg in C3H/HeSnJ. Examining the genetic
and developmental bases for variation in brain size within a species can
reveal bases for the large diversity in brain size found across species.
Variation in brain weight results primarily from differences in glia and
neuron number. This has been demonstrated by finding a high correlation
between brain weight and total brain DNA (Zamenhof and van Marthens, 1978).
Also, neocortical neuron number in humans is correlated with brain weight (r
= 0.56) and with neocortical volume (r = 0.69) (Pakkenberg and
Gundersen, 1997). Not surprisingly, estimates of neuron numbers for discrete
structures within the brain are also highly variable. For example, the
volume and cell number in the visual cortex of humans and other primates
ranges almost three-fold (Gilissen and Zilles, 1996; Suner and Rakic, 1996).
Large variation in neuron numbers have also been found among inbred mouse
strains in numerous cell populations, e.g., midbrain dopaminergic neurons
(Ross et al., 1976); forebrain cholinergic neurons (Albanese et al., 1985);
and granule cell number in hippocampus area dentata (Wimer et al., 1978).
Variation in neuron number is the result of both environmental and
genetic factors. A key question is how much of the variation is attributed
to the environment and how much is attributed to genetic factors. A
heritability estimate expresses the magnitude of the genetic contribution
and is measured as the ratio of the genetic variance over the total
phenotypic variance (VG/VP).
The heritability of brain size in humans has been estimated to be as high as
94% (Bartley et al., 1997). Heritability estimates for brain weight and
neuron number in mouse, are also high, 0.67 for brain weight (Roderick et
al., 1973) and 0.80 for granule cell number (Wimer and Wimer, 1989). These
high heritability estimates indicate that variation in brain size and neuron
number is generated predominantly by genetic variation. Identifying the
genetic variation that modulates neuron number can expose specific molecules
and the developmental and regulatory mechanisms controlling neuron number.
Forward genetic approach
The first step to determine the genetic bases of variation is to map the
genomic location of the genes responsible for the phenotypic variation.
Using phenotypic variation to map the responsible genes is known as the
forward genetic approach. One method for mapping a gene is linkage analysis.
The ability to perform linkage analysis requires genetic variation between
individuals. The genetic sites where DNA sequences differ between
individuals are called polymorphic loci, with each variant form representing
an allele type. Linkage analysis involves testing for an association between
the inheritance of a particular allele and the inheritance of a particular
phenotype. A strong association between an allele and a particular phenotype
indicates that a gene that contributes to the phenotypic variation maps near
the allele. The ability to use alleles as genetic markers to infer the
presence of a nearby gene is dependent on the physical linkage of loci on
chromosomes. The closer the physical proximity between two loci the less
likely they are to form a chiasmata between them and recombine with their
homologous chromosome during meiosis. The frequency that genetic loci
recombine translates into units of genetic distance called centimorgans (cM).
The frequency of recombination can be determined from the percentage of
non-parental allele combinations in the offspring.
Gene mapping before recombinant DNA technology required mutations that
exhibited prominent visible or biochemical phenotypes in the offspring to
carry out linkage analysis. For example, the first linkage demonstrated in
the mouse was between the albino (c) mutation and the pink-eyed
dilution (p) mutation (Haldane et al., 1915). In the last decade a
dense map of genetic markers called microsatellites and their ease of typing
by the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) has revolutionized genetic mapping.
Microsatellites or simple sequence repeats (SSR) consist of di- or
tri-nucleotide repeats, such as CACACA (Dietrich, 1992; Love, 1990) and are
dispersed at a high frequency throughout mammalian genomes. Over 6,000
microsatellites have been genetically mapped in the mouse and recently 4,000
of these markers were physically mapped (Dietrich et al., 1994).
Microsatellites have been typed in 12 inbred mouse strains and have been
found to be highly polymorphic between strains.
Gene mapping has been greatly facilitated by the production of genetic
variants produced by x-ray, chemical, and P–element induced mutations
(Sentry et al., 1994). In Drosophila and zebrafish, the production of
mutant phenotypes by mutagenesis and careful screening has lead to the
identification of many genes involved in neurogenesis (Sentry et al., 1994).
Large-scale mutagenesis screens are now underway in mouse and are expected
to yield a plethora of mutant phenotypes for study (Kasarskis et al., 1998).
However, one drawback of induced mutations that they have pleiotropic
effects, and many mutations will affect the viability of the animal, making
it difficult to study.
Exploiting natural genetic variation
Of the roughly 40,000 genes expressed in the human brain, approximately
10–20% are expected to have multiple alleles (Silver, 1995). Almost all of
the heritable variation between individuals results from random segregation
of different alleles that produce subtle quantitative effects (Falconer and
Mackay, 1996). A quantitative trait, such as height, has a continuous range
of phenotypes in a population and is controlled by multiple genetic and
environmental factors. In contrast, qualitative traits have distinct
phenotypes, such as coat color in mice, and are controlled by one or two
genes with little environmental influence. Genes with allelic variants that
produce quantitative variation are called quantitative trait loci (QTLs).
Quantitative traits such as neuron number can be used in a forward genetic
approach to identify the location of the QTLs. Mapping a QTL is different
from mapping a gene with qualitative expression in that there is no
one-to-one relation between a genotype and a phenotype. This is because the
effects of other genes and environmental variation combine to produce the
expressed phenotype. A QTL is mapped by comparing the phenotypic means from
the progeny in each genotype class at every marker across the genome.
Linkage is suggested when there is a significant difference between the
means of the genotypic classes.
Mice are the ideal animal for genetic analysis, especially if the goal is
to extrapolate to humans. Although, the evolutionary divergence of mice and
humans occurred over 60 million years ago, there is still a close
correspondence between the genomes of mice and humans. Large segments of the
human and mouse genomes are conserved and the differences found today can be
explained by breaking the mouse genome into roughly 130 segments and
shuffling them into a new order (Nadeau and Taylor, 1984). The existence of
a great variety of inbred mouse strains, which represent a wealth of
quantitative variation, is crucial in a quantitative genetic analysis. Mice
from a particular inbred strain are genetically identical or isogenic.
Phenotyping a number of isogenic mice to obtain a strain average can
minimize the variance due to environmental factors, measurement errors, or
developmental factors, resulting in a more accurate mean phenotype. Mice are
usually inbred by at least 20 generations of brother and sister mating.
Inbreeding forces the alleles to become homozygous at all loci. Finally, in
consideration of time and cost, mice reproduce quickly and are relatively
inexpensive to house.
Dissertation research
In my dissertation research, I use the forward genetic approach to assess
and dissect genetic sources of variation in neuron number. I have focused on
variation in neuron number on a global scale by using the surrogate measure
of whole brain weight. I have also focused on variation within a discrete
neuron population, the retinal ganglion cells. I began my studies by
determining the relative proportion of variance in brain weight and ganglion
cell number that is due to environmental and genetic factors (Chapters 2 and
4). Subsequently, I used linkage analysis with composite interval mapping
techniques to pursue QTLs that are responsible for genetic variation in
brain weight and ganglion cell number (Chapters 3 and 5). To identify the
developmental mechanisms generating variation in neuron number I have
examined the role of cell production and cell death in the generation of
ganglion cell variation among mouse strains (Chapter 6).
In the next section I address the behavioral significance of variation in
brain size and neuron number. Finally, I conclude this introduction with a
summary of molecular neural development to acquaint the reader with the
processes in which QTLs might be involved to produce variation in neuron
number.
Please note that although I present the work in Chapters 4 and 5 using
the first person singular pronoun, this work is actually the result of
collaboration with Robert Williams, Dan Goldowitz, and Dennis Rice. For the
most part, the methods and results in Chapters 4 and 5 match those in recent
published papers. Differences include the addition of new strains and larger
numbers of cases per strain and as a result I have also reanalyzed most of
the data.
Importance of neuron number
Brain weight
Brain tissue is metabolically costly and an increase in brain size should
be counterbalanced by an increased capacity for behavioral adaptations
(Williams and Herrup, 1988). Increased neuron number is usually associated
with more elaborate neural networks that have evolved for novel sensory or
behavioral adaptations required for exploiting new ecological niches. The
behavioral significance of variation in brain size is readily apparent
across species, where more complex behaviors are generally associated with
larger brains (Aboitiz, 1996). However, within species behavioral
associations with variation in brain size are less evident. Controversial
findings have been reported in humans, with positive correlation between
cerebral volume measured by MRI and IQ in children (Reiss et al., 1996), but
the absence of a correlation between forebrain volume, also measured by MRI,
and IQ in young adults (Tramo et al., 1998). The disparity may have to do
with the differences in the age of the subjects. Older subjects harbor the
effects of years of environmental variance, as well as an extended period of
brain growth, which could dissolve any initial correlation between brain
size and IQ.
In mice, high and low brain weight lines, generated by selective
breeding, initially showed behavioral differences in field activity, but
these differences disappeared in later generations (Wimer et al., 1969). A
recent study with rats reported a high correlation between general
intelligence and brain weight (Anderson, 1993). In summary, an association
with brain weight and behavior within a species is inconclusive and the
outcome of individual studies may depend on the particular structures
responsible for differences in brain size and the appropriateness of the
behavioral tests.
Discrete populations
Behavioral associations are found with the size of discrete neuronal
populations. The relative size of the hippocampus has been shown to be
associated with spatial memory capacity in species of passerines, cowbirds
(Healy and Krebs, 1996; Reboreda et al., 1996), and kangaroo rats (Jacobs
and Spencer, 1994). In males of many seasonally breeding songbird species
the volume of the song nuclei, hyperstriatum ventralis, pars caudalis (HVc)
and archistriatum (RA), increases along with their singing ability during
each breeding season (Smith et al., 1997). In male zebra finches,
neuron number and the volume of HVc and RA in an individual correlates
positively with the number of tutor syllables copied (Ward et al., 1998).
Moreover, in humans, cerebellum volume significantly correlates with the
individual’s verbal memory and fine motor dexterity (Paradiso et al., 1997).
In conclusion, there is a strong association between the number of neurons
in a discrete population and its behavioral output, particularly if
the behavior is important for survival.
Numbers of retinal ganglion cells range from fifty thousand in nocturnal
rodents to several million in diurnal birds and primates (Rager and Rager,
1978; Rakic and Riley, 1983). Variation is also marked within species:
numbers range from 0.7 to 1.5 million in humans (Curcio and Allen, 1990),
and from 40,000 to 80,000 in mice (Williams et al., 1996). It is not known
whether visual acuity differs in animals with high and low ganglion
cell number. However, visual acuity in glaucoma patients is compromised
after a small decrease in ganglion cell number (Quigley et al., 1989). The
difference in visual acuity will depend on what classes of ganglion cells
are variable. There are three major morphological classes of ganglion cells
in the mammalian retina (alpha, beta, and gamma)(Rodieck and Brening, 1983).
The extent of variability within the represented classes of ganglion cells
has not been studied. However, the numbers of beta and gamma cells are
likely to be the most variable since they are the most numerous, with each
representing roughly 45% to 50% of the ganglion cell population in several
species (Wässle and Boycott, 1991; Williams et al., 1993). The high density
and short dendritic fields of the gamma ganglion cell class are thought to
set the limit for spatial resolution (Wässle and Boycott, 1991) and thus,
even moderate variation in the density of this class should affect visual
acuity. The two-fold variation in ganglion cell number among mice is
significantly associated with retinal area (r = 67, t = 269,
P <0.001) (Rice et al., 1995), and thus cells per degree of visual
angle, the critical factor determining visual acuity, may not change with
increased cell number.
Developmental control of neuron number
Neuron number in the brain
The number of neurons in the adult central nervous system results from a
balance between cell proliferation and developmental cell death. While there
is considerable cell death in most vertebrate brains, the major process
defining brain size in a species is neurogenesis. Neural progenitors
originate from the embryonic ectoderm. The neural fate is the default
pathway for embryonic ectoderm. The switch from the neural fate into the
epidermal fate is initiated by an endogenous inhibitory signal within the
ectoderm, possibly BMP-4 (Hemmati-Brivanlou and Melton, 1997). Release from
the neural inhibitory signal may be mediated by follistatin, secreted from
the mesoderm.
Once cells are committed to the neural fate they coalesce to form the
neural plate. The neural plate then invaginates along the neuroaxis, folds
inward, and fuses to form the neural tube. In the neural tube,
neuroepithelial cells divide in the ventricular zone, leave the cell cycle,
and migrate outward to reside in the appropriate cell layers. In
neurogenesis, a complex molecular cascade controls the progressive hierarchy
of cell fate competence that ultimately leads to a specific cell type in the
appropriate place and numbers.
In mice, neocortical neurons are generated from a founding population in
the pseudostratified ventricular epithelium (PVE) between embryonic day 11
(E11) and E17. During this interval of neurogenesis there are an average of
eleven cell cycles, which increase the number in the founding population by
140 fold (Takahashi et al., 1995). At the onset of neurogenesis neural
progenitors undergo symmetrical divisions in which both daughter cells
return to the cell cycle (Rakic, 1988). During this time, cells increase in
number exponentially. As development proceeds the neural progenitors begin
to divide asymmetrically, with one daughter cell leaving the cell cycle and
differentiating while the other daughter cell continues to divide.
Gradually, as the neurogenesis proceeds, both daughter cells will cease to
divide and differentiate. The portion of the population that remains
proliferative represents the P fraction, and is equal to 1 when all cells
are dividing, while the portion of cells leaving the cell cycle represents
the Q fraction, for quiescent, and is equal to 1 when all cells have ceased
to divide (Takahashi et al., 1997).
Developmental mechanisms for variability. Variability in neuron
numbers could result from differences in the (1) number of founding neural
progenitors, (2) parameters of the cell cycle, and (3) duration of cell
proliferation. The variation of Purkinje cell number among chimeric mice
produced between wildtype mice and lurcher, a mouse mutant whose
Purkinje cell population is completely absent, appears to occur in quantum
units equal to 10,200 cells. The integral units suggest that variation in
Purkinje cell number results from differences in the initial proliferative
fraction of wildtype progenitors (Wetts and Herrup, 1983). Quantum
differences in the Purkinje cell number in chimeras made between the mouse
strains C57BL/6 and AKR/J has also demonstrated that variation can result
from differences in the initial clone size of progenitors (Herrup, 1986).
These studies suggest that differences in cell number can originate from
differences in the number of early progenitors, probably arising during
regionalization of the neural plate or neural tube.
Variability in parameters of the cell cycle. Variation in neuron
number could result from differences in the parameters of the cell cycle.
One such parameter is the rate at which proliferative fraction Q approaches
1, which could be a major factor in controlling the number of neurons
generated (Takahashi et al., 1997). In the mouse neocortex the Q fraction
equals 0.5 at cell cycle number 8. However, if the cell cycle at which Q
equals 0.5 were delayed from 8 to 10 the population would expand 5–fold. The
number of neurons generated is also determined by the progressive increase
in the length of the cell cycle. During the period of neocortical
neurogenesis in mouse, the length of the cell cycle increases from ~8.1
hours to ~18.4 hours. This increased cycle time is a result of a 4-fold
increase in the length of G1 (Takahashi et al., 1995). A broad range in the
length of G1 has been found among different cell populations (Jacobson,
1991).
In primates the striate visual cortex has a higher density of neurons
compared to other cortical areas. The differences in cell number between the
striate and extrastriate cortex was shown to result from a higher
proliferative fraction, but also from a higher portion of cells cycling, as
shown by 3H-thymidine pulse labeling (Dehay et al., 1993). The proliferative
portion, known as the labeling index, was 50% higher in the striate cortex
than in the extrastriate. The authors concluded that differences in the
mitotic rate must have produced the difference in the labeling index,
however an equally plausible explanation is a difference in the progression
of Q. Nevertheless, it is not known whether variability in the rate of Q or
variability in the length of the cell cycle contributes to variation in cell
number.
Variability in the number of cycles can have a large impact on cell
number. For example, if the number of cell cycles doubled while the relative
progression rate of P to Q was the same, the population would increase
60-fold. Of course, an increase in the cell cycle number, and consequently
the duration of neurogenesis, means that the over all progression of Q is
slower. It has been known for some time that the large differences in neuron
number found between species is best explained by differences in the period
of neurogenesis (Passingham, 1975). A recent study confirmed this finding by
finding a high correlation between the peak day of neurogenesis and the size
of 51 brain structures among 7 different mammals (Finlay and Darlington,
1995).
Extrinsic signals in cell cycle control. Although much is known about
cell cycle mechanics and the molecular components, little is known about the
regulatory mechanisms governing variation in the exit from the cell cycle
(Ross, 1996). In vertebrates the decision of a neural precursor to leave the
cell cycle during development involves the Notch signaling pathway (Beatus
and Lendahl, 1998). Notch, a transmembrane protein, and its ligands, Delta
and Serrate, are the mediators of cell-cell interactions that control the
competence of neighboring cells. Through a process called lateral
inhibition, a prospective neuron expressing Delta or Serrate on its cell
surface binds the Notch receptor on a neighboring cell thereby activating
the cell’s Notch pathway, which inhibits cell differentiation and holds them
in a proliferative state. Growth factors may regulate the expression of
genes in the Notch pathway. For example, the epidermal growth factor (EGF),
which can induce widespread proliferation of neuronal precursors in the
brain (Kuhn et al., 1997), was recently shown in the developing retina to
down regulate Mash-1, a proneural factor within the Notch signaling pathway
(Ahmad et al., 1998). A connection between EGF and Notch suggests that one
way growth factors may act is by down regulating the Notch pathway, thereby
holding cells in a proliferative state. Other extrinsic factors such as
retinoic acid and thyroid hormone influence the extent of cell proliferation
in the brain by inducing the withdrawal from the cell cycle and subsequent
differentiation (Jacobson, 1991; Kelley et al., 1994). How the expression
patterns and concentration levels of specific growth factors and hormones
are regulated to set proliferative limits to defined populations of neurons
is not known.
Finally, extrinsic factors ultimately regulate the progression of the
cell cycle by inhibiting or promoting the build up of cyclins and the
activity of their dependent kinases (Hutchinson and Glover, 1995). The
complete pathway between the proliferative effects of growth factors and
hormones and their effectors within the cell cycle during neurogenesis
remain to be mapped out. In summary, the developmental control of neuron
number involves a complex collaboration of molecular pathways that converge
to determine the progression of the cell cycle through specific checkpoints.
Retinal ganglion cell development
In mouse the neural retina is part of the brain, comprising ~0.4% of the
brain’s weight. The eyes begin to form in the mouse around E9 with the
bilateral out-pocketing of the forebrain into a structure called the optic
vesicle (Mann, 1964; Pei and Rhodin, 1970). The optic vesicle grows outward
toward the surface ectoderm and then invaginates to form the optic cup. The
optic cup then separates into an inner portion, the neural retina and an
outer portion, the pigment epithelium.
Proliferating retinal cells migrate in a to-and-fro pattern, moving to
the inner retina to synthesize DNA and then back to the outer retina to
divide. During retinal development the percentage of proliferating cells (P)
decreases as cells from asymmetric divisions leave the cell cycle, migrate
toward the vitreal surface and differentiate. The neural retina develops
into three distinct cell layers, a ganglion cell layer, a bipolar layer and
an inner photoreceptor layer. The onset of differentiation for retinal cells
occurs in a temporally-ordered manner, with retinal ganglion cells, amacrine
cells, and horizontal cells beginning to differentiate early, while
photoreceptors, bipolar cells and Müller glia cells begin to differentiate
later (Sidman, 1961). Although the generation of cell types begin and peak
at different times the cell types are being generated simultaneously
throughout most of the developmental period. In mouse, ganglion cells start
to differentiate on E11 and continue until just before birth (Dräger, 1985).
Developmental cell death of ganglion cells begins at, or just before,
birth, peaks between postnatal days 4–6, and is essentially complete by P12
(Linden and Pinto, 1985; Young, 1984). Variation in ganglion cell number
could result from differences in the extent of ganglion cell production or
ganglion cell death.
Variability in ganglion cell production. Variation in ganglion cell
production could result from differences in: (1) numbers of retinal
progenitor cells, (2) the kinetics and duration of progenitor cell
proliferation, or (3) bias in cell fate determination in progenitors. The
variance of retinal cell numbers in embryonic chick to peaked during the
exponential phase of cell division and were lowest after cell
differentiation (Morris and Cowan, 1984). A regression analysis of the
variance in cell number at the start of incubation and growth rate indicated
that the variation in the exponential phase was due mostly to the variance
in the number of retinal progenitors at the start of incubation. However,
variation in retinal cell number at the start of incubation could result
from subtle differences in the correspondence of the developmental stage and
not necessarily to the variability in the number of retinal progenitors
Variance in numbers of multipotent retinal progenitors should have
consistent effects on the number of many retinal cell types. However, a
comparison of horizontal cell and ganglion cell numbers for six strains has
demonstrated that ratios in these early-generated cell types are not always
matched (Williams et al., 1998). In fact, preliminary data indicates a weak
negative correlation exists between the number of ganglion cells and
photoreceptors within inbred strains (Williams et al., 1998). If true, that
would suggest that a reciprocal relationship exists between early- and
late-generated retinal cell types and that variation in ganglion cell number
is produced by differentiation bias. An example of the reciprocal
relationship between retinal cell types is found when the inhibition of
Notch signaling results in an increase of early-generated cell types at the
expense of later-generated cell types (Dorsky et al., 1997). It is possible
that a genetic bias in the Notch signaling pathway may play a role in
executing the differentiation ratios of retinal cell types.
Variation in the ganglion cell number could result from differences in
the proliferation kinetics of progenitors that give rise to ganglion cells.
The rates of mitosis may be influenced through inhibitory molecules and
pathways. One example is dopa—a tyrosine metabolite that normally has
inhibitory effects on cell genesis in the retina. The absence of dopa in
albino rats leads to an anomalous up-regulation of ganglion cell production
followed by an increase in the severity of cell death (Ilia and Jeffery,
1999). Finally, as with brain weight, large differences between
species within a defined population is best explained by differences in the
duration of neurogenesis. For example, a two-fold difference in the duration
of retinal neurogenesis can explain the near two-fold difference in retinal
ganglion cell number between gerbil and hamster (Wikler et al., 1989).
Variability in ganglion cell death. Differences in cell death can
produce large differences in cell number across species. Estimates of
retinal ganglion cell death range from none at all in fish to 40% in
chicken, 50–60% in rat (Lam et al., 1982), and up to 80% in cat (Williams et
al., 1986). The ganglion cell population in the domestic cat is 30% lower
than that of wildcat —the species from which the domestic cat has evolved.
Retinal ganglion cell number in a Spanish wildcat fetus and domestic cat
fetus obtained at E38, were remarkably close, suggesting that cell death
rather than cell production generates the marked species differences
(Williams et al., 1993).
Variation in the severity of cell death may result from differences in
titers of neurotrophic factors. The neurotrophins—BDNF and
neurotrophin-3/4—have been found to increase survival of retinal ganglion
cells in chicken and rat (Ma et al., 1998; Rosa et al., 1993). Neuregulin,
found on the cell surface and as a secreted protein, can also increase
survival of neonatal rat retinal ganglion cells (Bermingham-McDonogh
et al., 1996). Differences in the concentration or expression time of these
neurotrophic factors, their receptors, or components within their signaling
pathways could produce variation in the severity of naturally occurring
ganglion cell death.
In summary, the generation of variation in cell number could involve any
of the molecular processes described above. Determining the genetic bases of
variation in neuron number could identify key regulatory genes in
neurogenesis, which may lead to a greater understanding of cell-cycle
regulation. In addition, examining the molecular variants in the QTLs that
produce natural variation in neuron number will lead to a better
understanding of the genetic bases of quantitative variation and provide
insight into the molecular nature of brain evolution.
Return to Table of Contents
|
|
|